Join Us

E-Books vs. Print: What Parents Need to Know

When Maggie Moore, a suburban Denver mom, was literally weighing her packing options for a family trip, she was stumped by her 4-year-old son’s stack of favorite books. He had dozens, and she knew they’d be too heavy to take along. But they would be away for a few weeks — how could she bring only a few?

That’s when she reluctantly bought a Nook, loading titles for both of them onto it. She wasn’t a big fan of the extra screen time it would mean for her preschooler, but Moore justified the purchase as a stopgap solution. What happened next surprised her: From the moment her son held the device and began to scroll through a book, he was transfixed. “He was in heaven,” says Moore.

Once products targeted primarily at adults, e-reading devices have expanded to include a younger audience: children.

“We are not going to stop this train,” says psychologist Jim Taylor, Ph.D., author of Raising Generation Tech. But should we try to slow it down? When it comes to the youngest readers, some experts are skittish about putting tablets into tiny hands. Parents are conflicted, too — 68 percent prefer that their 6- to 8-year-olds read print books, Scholastic found. Since there’s not much research out there, it may be years before we understand the impact of tech devices on young readers.

Still, there are signs that e-readers can have a positive effect on newbie readers, especially when it comes to targeted learning based on each child’s ability. But don’t give those storybooks the heave-ho just yet. “It doesn’t have to be an either-or. You don’t build a house with only one tool,” says Otis Kriegel, a fifth-grade teacher in New York City and the author of Covered in Glue: What New Elementary School Teachers Really Need to Know. Here’s how you can inspire your reader with both options. 

PLUS: 7 BOOKS TO TEACH KIDS ABOUT DIVERSITY 

Print May Be Better For...

The hands-on experience. Some experts, including Taylor, worry that devices can distance little kids from the real world. If they’re only exposed to e-readers, kids lose the tactile experience of handling a traditional book, turning its pages, or sharing their faves with friends. “Technology is a beautiful box but it is still a box,” he says.

Falling in love with reading. Cuddling with a parent over a book or gathering around the teacher for storytime helps kids associate reading with nurturing. “These reading experiences can set the stage for later reading success,” says Julia Parish-Morris, a post-doctoral fellow at the University of Pennsylvania who studies how young children interact with e-books. While she thinks e-books are great for independent readers, she’s not as sure how good they are for preschoolers and kindergartners. Her research has found that parents often become more controlling, concentrating more on what their child is doing with the device instead of talking about the story.

Focusing a child's attention. The music, animation, and games that are loaded into kids’ e-books can end up being more distracting than useful, says Lisa Guernsey, director of the early education initiative at the New America Foundation. “The technology is so exciting that the conversation focuses on what button to push instead of the content,” she says. What’s better is when those bells and whistles lead back to the story, instead of just entertaining.

PLUS: AUTHOR'S TIPS ON HOW TO GET KIDS TO LOVE TO READ

Digital Matches Print For...

Boosting early reading skills. For the past four years the Center for Literacy at the University of Akron has been studying how to integrate e-readers into classrooms. Jeremy Scott Brueck, director of the school’s Digital Text Initiative, found that animation and audio in e-books did seem to help young kids identify printed words. When Brueck tested pre-K students, a third knew the words before reading the story with a grown-up on an e-reader. After reading the e-book, the number shot up to 54 percent. It’s unclear whether the results would have come out the same with traditional books; it might have been the shared reading experience — a known vocabulary-builder — rather than the device that helped kids learn. But what they did find: “The kids were extremely engaged,” says Brueck.

Digital May Trump Print Because...

It's more interactive. While add-ons can distract, they are extremely useful for beginning readers, who can zoom in on unfamiliar words or click links that help make connections to their world, says Guernsey. Plus, the touchscreen or buttons on an e-reader can hone a preschooler’s fine-motor skills. 

It's more rewarding. When kids see printed words light up as they sound out the words, they’re encouraged. Kim Floyd has been teaching kindergarten in Napa Valley, CA, for 24 years and using iPads loaded with books for the last three. The proof of e-reading success is in front of her every day when she sees how excited her students are the second she pulls out the tablets. Because the devices help children understand words by highlighting and defining those they struggle with, their vocabulary increases. Her kindergartners have vocabularies more typical of second graders, she notes. Floyd even studied the vocab-building phenomena for her master’s degree last year, testing pre-kindergartners who were not native English speakers and had no preschool experience. By the end of three weeks, their vocabularies had jumped from roughly 200 words into the thousands.

It caters to a kid's unique learning style. Floyd also likes that the anonymity of the device helps struggling readers feel less embarrassed. “It lets children find a book that fits their interest and skill without the entire class knowing what they are reading,” she says. Erika Alexander, a suburban Detroit mother, agrees. Her fourth-grade son is a reluctant reader, even though books were part of his routine when he was younger. Recently when they were shopping, he picked up a Nook that was loaded with a graphic novel. Attracted at first to the gadgetry, he stood in the aisle and inhaled the story. Alexander still plans to encourage a love of old-fashioned books. But she also recognizes that her son is a visual person, and a high-tech device hooks him in ways that were missing before.

The Bottom Line...

Kids have a lot to gain from both reading tools. Even though she’s a huge e-reader fan, Floyd believes that children should be exposed to print first or at least simultaneously. Her students switch off easily, and there are surprisingly few squabbles over who gets the iPad. “After the novelty wears off, they become nonchalant,” she says. Plus, technology will never replace good parenting and good teachers. So when you read to your child — regardless of whether it’s a traditional or e-book — keep the conversation lively. Talk about what he sees on the page. Ask what he thinks will happen next. Because as researchers and educators all agree, the most important app, especially for little kids, is human.

PLUS: HOW TO GET YOUR CHILD TO READ CONSISTENTLY 

Young children’s use of digital devices is increasing as we progress through the 21st century and handheld and mobile devices, such as smartphones and tablets, have become increasingly available. While older children using tablets to read has been more broadly investigated, less is known about the impacts of digital reading on children at the stage of literacy acquisition. An analytical review was conducted on the effects of interactive e-book interventions for young children’s literacy development when compared to (a) listening to print books, (b) regular school programs, and (c) reading non-enhanced and non-interactive e-books. A significant additional beneficial effect of e-book interventions was found for phonological awareness and vocabulary learning based on data from 1138 children in 14 randomized controlled trial (RCT) studies. When e-books are properly selected and used, children develop literacy skills equally well and sometimes better than with print books. Additionally, e-book interventions outperformed the regular school program in the development of literacy skills. Similarly, enhanced e-book conditions revealed benefits over the non-enhanced e-book interventions in literacy skill acquisition. The impact of these findings related to health issues, e-book design, disadvantaged populations, and adult-led e-book sharing is discussed.

The present analytical review used more stringent inclusion criteria than previous reviews and meta-analyses, only including randomized controlled trial studies (RCTs) that have a control group and a pre- and post-intervention design, including baseline measures. Meta-analytic techniques to control for the effects of statistical dependence on effect sizes were used. However, due to the scarce number of RCTs on this topic and the differences in methodologies and excess of variables involved in the reviewed papers, it was decided to exclude meta-analysis procedures. However, the present study could be used as a precursor to a meta-analysis.

The aims of this paper are to (a) explore how effective e-books are for supporting early literacy skills: Concepts about print, phonological awareness, and vocabulary as well as reading comprehension; (b) examine the effects of e-book reading interventions compared to printed book reading; (c) study the results of enhanced e-book reading interventions compared to non-enhanced e-book reading interventions or simple static e-books; (d) study the effects of e-book reading interventions compared to the regular school program; and (e) analyze the reviewed studies from adult support, e-book design, and disadvantaged populations. We were specifically interested in the additional effect on literacy development of interactive or enhanced e-book interventions as compared to the more traditional presentation of stories, such as reading a print story book, following the regular literacy curriculum, or reading a more traditional e-book without enhanced characteristics.

Given these gaps in our understanding of the extent to which e-books support early emergent reading skills, this study is dedicated to e-book-based interventions for the acquisition of literacy skills of children from 4 to 7 years old, including studies from 2010 to 2020. We consider an e-book as any form of electronic text that contains key features of print books. This synthesis includes both traditional e-books (static digital version of a print book) and enhanced e-books (with the key features of a traditional print book, but with animated and interactive content, audio, and video). This definition of an e-book excludes other software that can support literacy development, but is not embedded in a book format.

Previous reviews [ 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 ] and meta-analyses [ 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 ] have broken ground into furthering our understanding of how e-books can enhance literacy skills and practice. However, none of these reviews have been conducted for investigating the effects of e-book use on the reading outcomes of preschool, kindergarten, and first grade students in the last 10 years. There is a lack of knowledge concerning digital reading with regard to the extent to which e-book interventions support different aspects of a student’s emergent literacy acquisition and to which enhanced features within e-books improve literacy outcomes. There is a pressing tension between the actions that could be taken and the actions that should be taken with e-books [ 14 ].

The current review is an attempt to better understand the effects of e-book use on reading outcomes for children aged 7 years or younger. This specific age group was chosen based on previous research findings, suggesting that book reading in the first 6 years of life is related to emergent reading and reading achievement [ 36 ].

Considering these two scenarios, young children growing up in contexts saturated with technology and the shift away from the classroom and the adoption of online and digital learning in many parts of the world, policies and recommendations must evolve and provide thoughtful, practical advice to parents and teachers founded on evidence, and not based merely on the precautionary principle [ 35 ].

It is reasonable for parents and teachers to be concerned about the excessive use of digital content, especially in young children. However, touch screen devices are rapidly gaining place in the lives of families with young children, and parents also hold positive views toward technology use and are able to identify a range of benefits that their children have acquired [ 1 , 2 ]. Today, media represent just another environment; children do the same things they have always done, only virtually.

Moreover, since the cognitive control mechanisms are still immature in young children [ 29 ] the high exposure to digital games (also found in e-books) makes them especially vulnerable to develop pathological gaming behavior [ 30 ]. Pathological gaming has become a major concern for health care professionals during the last years [ 31 ] and has been included as a game disorder in the International Classification of Disease (ICD-11) Manual [ 32 ], as well as, a condition called Internet Gaming disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5 (DSM-5) [ 33 ]. Thus, evidence is sufficient to recommend time limitations on digital media use for children 2 to 5 years to no more than 1 h per day [ 34 ].

Heavy media use during preschool years is associated with negative effects on children’s health, general development, and outdoor play [ 1 ]. The risks of children spending a lot of time in front of a screen have been well documented by research: Addiction [ 25 ], obesity [ 26 ], negative effects on motor dexterity [ 27 ], and eye fatigue [ 28 ], among others.

Despite these concerns, previous empirical studies have identified the effects that e-books can have on the development of children’s literacy skills. Due to their many unique features, e-books provide children with many opportunities for promoting their emergent literacy skills. For instance, studies have shown that digital books support the development of children’s print and phonological awareness [ 18 , 19 , 20 ], vocabulary development [ 18 , 21 , 22 ], spelling development [ 23 ], and reading comprehension [ 5 , 24 ]. These skills (e.g., phonological awareness, print awareness, vocabulary, and reading comprehension) are considered significant to the development of children’s emergent literacy abilities.

Another finding of the literature on e-book reading is that adult–child interaction and e-book sharing with young children differs from sharing print books [ 8 , 10 , 11 ]. Parents reported their children not only read traditional books more than electronic books, but enjoyed them more and paid more attention to them. Caregivers also reported participating in more talk about the story when reading print books than electronic books [ 17 ]. Similarly, teachers sharing an enhanced digital book struggle to define their role [ 6 ]. When sharing a digital book, children may be occupied by the interactive elements in the book (tapping hotspots initiates sounds, simple animations, and dialogue/sounds from the characters) while ignoring the story. More research is needed to further explore new routines that develop when families or educators have access to a set of well-designed digital picture books.

Despite the positive outcomes of e-book reading, a number of researchers have taken a more critical view on e-books due to their incorporation of features such as hotspots that contain animation, sound, and other multimedia effects and may distract young readers from the story content and negatively affect their understandings of the story’s main theme [ 10 , 11 , 13 , 14 ]. Some hotspots are congruent with the story (i.e., support the story’s content) and others are incongruent (i.e., they do not align with the story content and might even distract the reader from it) [ 4 ]. In this sense, a recent analysis on e-book design reveals that the first published digital books included hotspots that often had little or no relevance to the story and distracted children from language and literacy learning. Nevertheless, important improvements have been made in e-book design compared to former years as the number of interactive visuals and of hotspots seem much lower than in previous years, and they are more congruent; that is, they elaborate or extend the story line, as it is advisable [ 16 ].

Two of these reviews [ 8 , 9 ] concluded that e-books and printed books play different roles in learning to read and, therefore, the false dichotomy between these two forms of reading should be eliminated, because they are different experiences of reading. A review investigating the role of digital reading [ 9 ] found that tablets may improve emergent literacy skills. However, parent or teacher scaffolding is needed to maximize the benefits of e-books. A different review [ 10 ] reported that well-designed e-books are as effective as printed books in improving reading acquisition outcomes. A meta-analysis on multimedia stories, which compared independent e-book reading with traditional shared book reading, found that multimedia features can provide similar scaffolding to reading with an adult [ 12 ]. A different meta-analysis [ 13 ] found that stories presented through multimedia can support and even strengthen children’s understanding of the story compared to listening to stories in more traditional settings, such as storybook reading. Similarly, a research synthesis of experimental and quasi-experimental studies investigating the effects of e-books on children’s literacy development found moderate effects of e-books on reading comprehension [ 14 ]. However, a recently published meta-analysis examining the effects of e-book use on literacy outcomes found no statistically significant effects between e-book and non-e-book conditions on norm-referenced standardized test measures of reading and reading comprehension [ 15 ].

In the last decade, five reviews [ 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 ] and four meta-analyses [ 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 ] have been carried out to compare children’s reading acquisition ability when using digital devices versus the use of traditional printed books [ 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 ].

However, questions regarding young children engaging with e-books arise regarding whether these digital stories are as beneficial for children as print books read by an adult. There has been much hope for the educational potential of interactive media, such as e-books, along with fear about their overuse during early childhood, a period of rapid brain development. We are only beginning to understand what this digital shift means for young children’s early literacy development [ 3 ]. The following questions are still unresolved: What do e-books bring to a child’s early literacy experience? Additionally, what are the digital reading potentials for improving and enriching the literacy environment of a young child?

E-books present interactive multimodal information as written text, oral reading, music, illustrations, animations, and hotspots that are activated by touching or pressing the touch screen to generate sound and animation (see an example of the e-book “A Shiver of Sharks”: http://bit.ly/2nM3Gr8 ) (accessed on 15 April 2021) [ 4 ]. With digitization, new opportunities for the mediation of multimodal text have emerged. Among the potential advantages of e-books is that they are easily accessible and interactive for beginning readers who cannot yet decode text or are just beginning to learn to decode. Even children with emergent literacy skills who cannot yet read can explore e-books by themselves without the help of an adult [ 5 ]. This invites reading practices that may differ from traditional book reading, due to the affordances of the digital touch screen and the social settings in which it is used [ 6 ].

Children are growing up in a digital media environment where interactions with digital media are an increasing part of children’s daily lives in classrooms and at home. More children, across all levels of society, are using interactive and mobile media on a daily basis [ 1 ]. In a recent survey of parents of children aged 8 years and under, the majority (98%) reported that they live in a home with some type of mobile device [ 2 ]. As a result of this exposure to technology, children today have many opportunities to explore digital devices and play with them. Many activities in children’s lives are digital, including early literacy experiences. Children’s books are increasingly available in a digital format on electronic devices—often handheld and mobile [ 3 ].

All 14 studies included in this review provided adequate statistical information (e.g., the pre- and post-intervention control and treatment group means, standard deviations, and numbers of participants in each group) that made it possible to calculate the effect sizes for the differences among different intervention, control, or treatment groups on literacy outcomes (e.g., concepts about print, phonological awareness, vocabulary, and reading comprehension). We applied the effect size formula based on the mean pre–post change in the treatment group minus the mean pre–post change in the control group (or a different treatment condition), divided by the pooled pretest standard deviation [ 40 ]. In order to reduce the bias estimation of effect sizes, a bias correction parameter, which considered the sample size of the two groups, was also applied, as recommended in [ 52 ]. This formula was chosen because (a) it is recommended for studies with repeated measurements in both the treatment and control groups, as is the case of the present study and (b) in terms of bias, precision, and robustness to heterogeneity of variance, it is favored over alternate effect size estimates [ 46 ]. Based on recommendations, estimates of less than 0.20 were considered non-significant; estimates of 0.20–0.49 were considered small effects; 0.50–0.79, medium effects; and values above 0.80, large effects [ 53 ].

The characteristics of the 14 reviewed studies are presented in , and . presents the conditions comparing exposure to e-books and printed books; shows comparisons between different types and conditions of e-books; and shows comparison between exposure to e-books and to the regular school program. The studies were coded by the three authors of the present study; 94% interrater agreement was achieved, and discrepancies in coding were resolved via discussion. We included general codes of the studies (e.g., author, study design, and participant information), as well as specific codes for e-book use (e.g., intervention description and device used to access the e-book, number and type of e-books, intervention duration, etc.).

The initial search yielded 2746 studies. After importing abstracts using Mendeley and removing duplicates, 1978 studies remained; after reviewing the abstracts, 106 studies were selected as possible studies to be included. Next, authors conducted a full-text review of the 106 remaining articles, resulting in a final sample of 14 studies that met the inclusion criteria. A PRISMA flowchart is shown in [ 39 ].

We searched for relevant published articles from January 2010 to January 2021. Literature searches were conducted in 9 databases: ERIC, Education Database, Psychology Database, Google Scholar database, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Medline, PubMed, and Social Science Database. Search terms included the following: electronic book, digital book, e-book, multimedia book and literacy, reading, reading comprehension, vocabulary, phonological awareness, and random controlled trial. Combinations of these terms were used to search for the targeted articles.

The study contained at least one control group or different treatment conditions. This included studies with passive or “business as usual” control groups and studies with active control groups, including control groups exposed to print book reading and to non-enhanced conditions of e-book reading;

The study contained at least one literacy outcome measure that yielded an objective quantitative score. These outcome measures included mainly scores on author-designed assessments, such as testing children’s knowledge of the vocabulary to which they had been exposed in the course of intervention;

The study contained a targeted e-book reading intervention. This included interventions that used e-book reading to target specific literacy skills; it also included interventions with kindergarten, preschool, and first grade age children; interventions delivered by researchers, teachers, or parents; and interventions delivered on a one-to-one basis or to children in groups;

In order to conduct the present review, we have followed the methodological approach to systematic reviews by Hammersley [ 37 ] where exhaustive searching for relevant material, transparent methodological assessment of studies, and a formal and explicit method for the synthesis of findings, have to be explicitly established.

Six studies compared e-books with the regular school program in seven different conditions and a total of 17 reported outcomes (see ). Three of these outcomes were reported on concepts about print, one of them with a negligible size; one with a medium size, and one with a large size. Four outcomes were reported on phonological awareness skills, one of them non-significant and three of them with medium size effects ranging from 0.52 to 0.54 in favor of the e-book condition. One of the conditions that favored the learning of phonological awareness skills showed children, when they clicked on a word, a large colorful frame displaying its phonetic segments in enlarged letters on the computer screen [ 19 , 51 ]. Studies reported 10 outcomes in vocabulary; eight of them had large effect sizes, in favor of the e-book. Conditions that favored the vocabulary outcomes were, for example, children listening to the audio of the story and engaged independently with the story activities, following verbal instructions that were part of the e-book [ 21 ], or children independently reading the static e-book versus playing computer games [ 48 ]. One outcome was in favor of the e-book condition with a medium size effect, and one non-significant outcome was reported for the PPVT test [ 49 ].

Seven studies compared enhanced e-books to non-enhanced e-books in 12 different conditions and a total of 26 reported outcomes (see ). Three of these outcomes were on phonological awareness, with effects ranging from non-significant to small size effects (0.15 to 0.48) in favor of enhanced conditions of e-book use, such as embedded metacognitive guidance in an educational e-book, which was found to support phonological awareness versus using an e-book without any metacognitive guidance [ 47 ]. Studies reported 21 outcomes on vocabulary, three of them with negligible effect sizes. Seven of them had small effect sizes ranging from ES = 0.26 to ES = 0.48. Four of them had medium effect sizes ranging from ES = 0.50 to ES = 0.74, and four of them had large effect sizes. All of them were in favor of the enhanced e-book conditions. Two of the reported outcomes concerning vocabulary were on the PPVT test [ 49 ] with non-significant effect sizes. The conditions that favored the learning of vocabulary were, for example, reading the e-book with adult vocabulary support versus dynamic visual vocabulary support embedded in the e-book [ 24 ]; visual vocabulary support versus no vocabulary support [ 24 ]; reading interactive animated e-books versus reading static e-books [ 48 ]; or embedded questions during the story versus embedded questions after the story [ 50 ]. Finally, two outcomes were reported on reading comprehension with large effect sizes, ES = 0.83 and ES = 0.9. The conditions that favored reading comprehension outcomes were teacher-delivered e-books with the 3R strategy “read, review, and recite” and keyword cues versus the same strategy without key words cues or the recite phase [ 46 ].

Five studies compared e-books to print book in nine different conditions with a total of 17 reported outcomes (see ). Two of the outcomes were on concepts about print, one with no significant effect and the other with a large effect of ES = 3.35 in favor of the e-book. Six of these outcomes were on phonological awareness with effects ranging from non-significant to small effect sizes (0.08 to 0.49) in favor of e-book use. Studies reported eight additional outcomes on vocabulary, one of them with negligible effects on students’ vocabulary, two of them with small-to-large effects (0.30 and 0.91) in favor of the print book use; in these two cases, the print book was read, and activities were guided by an adult, while the e-book condition was operated independently by the children. However, the other five outcomes on vocabulary in favor of the e-book use ranged from medium-to-large effect sizes (0.65 to 1.57). The conditions that favored the learning of vocabulary using the e-book versus the printed book used an enhanced e-book condition with vocabulary instructions versus listening to an adult-read print story with incidental vocabulary exposure [ 42 ] or using an e-book with scaffolding-like questions versus the experimenter reading aloud the print story one-on-one without receiving practice of the story [ 44 ]. In one of the conditions, the e-book was used only in the “read to me” mode versus the print book read by the experimenter without receiving any other practice; the outcomes favored the e-book [ 44 ]. Studies reported one outcome on reading comprehension in favor of the print book with a medium effect size, ES = 0.58.

The frequency of non-significant, small, medium, and large ES would be reported in each of the three studied conditions and on each literacy measure (see for a summary of the frequency of different ES in vocabulary and phonological awareness in the e-book and control conditions).

Measures created by the researcher, as opposed to norm-referenced standardized tests, are likely to be sensitive to growth in specific vocabulary [ 55 ]. As such, these measures are dependent on e-book text and should have greater outcomes than independent non-related e-book text measures. Additionally, interventions in the selected studies were not of a sufficient time duration to lead to changes in norm-referenced standardized tests or non-dependent measures of an e-book text.

In the reviewed studies, reading knowledge was mainly assessed using measures, created by the researcher, of target text, words, or sounds instructed or included in the e-books selected for the respective studies. In many of the studies, the tasks were based on existing tests (i.e., adjusted for the research, such as a created picture vocabulary test, a frequently used test format for young children’s vocabulary learning) [ 24 , 41 , 44 , 45 , 46 ].

Specially constructed electronic versions were adapted by researchers to fit or create a story adequate to the cultural context [ 21 ] and to capture educational principles, especially those supporting literacy development. In some cases, children were offered a number of interactive modes of operation (i.e., read only, read story with dictionary, and read the story and play) [ 20 ] and different versions (i.e., with metacognitive guidance and the other without such guidance) [ 47 ]. Among the salient characteristics of some of the selected e-books was the “read story with dictionary”, a mode embedded in the e-book, which offers an oral reading of the text together with explanations of difficult words. To support individualized vocabulary acquisition, young readers could click the hotspots and access the respective screens as often as they wished. Each difficult word was enunciated clearly by the narrator as it appeared on the screen and was associated with pictures that support its meaning [ 20 , 47 , 51 ].

The story books selected for the studies were both commercial e-books and e-books adapted by the researcher. In most cases, when authors selected a commercial e-book, their choice was based on a set of criteria, such as kindergarten teacher recommendations [ 41 ]; the inclusion of colorful pictures and a basic storyline appropriate for preschool and kindergarten children [ 42 ]; a good narrative structure and readability that provided descriptions of characters, settings, times, goals/initiating events, problems, and solutions/endings [ 24 , 44 ]; high-quality illustrations; ease of interaction (e.g., hot spots that were clearly identified); and interactive elements that did not interfere with the story text [ 39 ].

All selected studies were published in English, but conducted in different languages and countries: Six in Israel [ 19 , 20 , 24 , 41 , 47 , 51 ]; three in the US [ 42 , 44 , 45 ]); two in The Netherlands [ 48 , 50 ]; one in Canada [ 43 ]; one in South Africa [ 21 ]; and one in Taiwan [ 46 ].

Across the studies, four papers [ 19 , 20 , 21 , 47 ] reported a total of 330 students at risk for learning disabilities. A total of 24 students who were English Language Learners (ELLs) were reported in one paper [ 42 ], and the number of low-SES children reported in one paper was 78 [ 51 ].

In the 14 studies selected, a total of 1138 students were included with a mean age 63,92 months (with a mean age range between 52 and 82.2 months of age)—492 preschoolers, 507 kindergartens, and 139 first graders. The studies included 451 girls, 501 boys, and 186 students whose gender was not reported.

Four of the reviewed studies reported atypically large ES [ 20 , 21 , 48 , 51 ] if we compare them with the rest of the reviewed studies or previous research [ 12 , 13 , 14 ]. Ioannidis [ 54 ] points out that a single study cannot be seen in isolation and should be compared with the entire prior evidence on the same or similar questions. Another reason of atypically large ES, pointed by Ionnidis [ 54 ] could be a paradigm shift. In this regard, it is important to note that in the present review, most of the studies reporting large ES are in the “e-book vs. regular school program condition”. It could be inferred that children learning vocabulary with e-book interventions learn much more specific words than those not receiving any intervention at all. However, due to both random errors and biases, too large effect sizes require extra caution [ 54 ].

Overall, 14 studies met the inclusion criteria. A total of 11 studies were conducted in the last five years (i.e., 2014–2018). The remaining were published between 2011 and 2013. Studies were classified based on (1) e-books versus printed books ( and ), enhanced e-book versus non-enhanced e-book (i.e., interactive versus static e-books), ( ), and (2) e-book versus the regular school program ( ). Some of the selected studies contributed to more than one of these categories. The reading outcomes were classified into four variables: Concepts about print, phonological awareness (including measures of letter sound and letter name), vocabulary (including measures of receptive and expressive vocabulary, as well as other related measures of vocabulary, such as new words in story retelling, sentence completion, and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT [ 49 ]), and reading comprehension. The 14 studies included in the review contributed more than one effect size as a result of using multiple outcome measures or comparing more than one pair of conditions (i.e., multiple treatments and/or multiple comparison groups), resulting in a total of 60 effect sizes for the analysis. Of the 60 outcomes, 5 were on concepts about print, 13 were on phonological awareness, 39 on vocabulary, and 3 on reading comprehension (see ).

4. Discussion

This review provides a comprehensive assessment of a random control trial (RCT) and research evidence published in the last 10 years on the effectiveness of e-books on emergent literacy skills for children in preschool, kindergarten, and first grade. Our findings, although preliminary due to the limited scope of the available literature, provide researchers and practitioners with information for making evidence-based instructional decisions that can inform future research.

The primary objective of the present review was to examine the effectiveness of e-books for supporting early literacy skills, focusing on concepts about print, phonological awareness, vocabulary, and reading comprehension. Additionally, we explored the contributions of e-books compared to three conditions: Print books, e-books in non-enhanced conditions, and the regular school program.

With regard to the first aim on the effectiveness of e-book interventions for supporting literacy skills for young children, two outcomes out of five on concepts about print were found to produce large effects in favor of e-book interventions. Furthermore, modest gains were observed in the 13 outcomes on phonological awareness in favor of e-book conditions (see ). Additionally, out of 39 outcomes on vocabulary, 7 of them were found to produce small effect sizes, and 21 of them were found to produce medium-to-large effects; that is, 78% of outcomes in vocabulary clearly favored the e-book condition (see ). Finally, out of the 14 studies, only three outcomes were reported on reading comprehension, and two of them were found to produce large size effects in favor of e-book interventions. In considering the effectiveness of e-books for supporting literacy skills in young children, we would like to highlight three key conclusions.

First, as in previous reviews [8,11] and meta-analyses [12,13], we found evidence that e-book interventions have a significant positive effect on measures of vocabulary when used by young readers compared to print books and regular school programs. Vocabulary performance was also higher in the enhanced e-book condition versus the non-enhanced e-book one. Among the key characteristics of e-book reading that could have a positive effect on vocabulary learning is research attention to the interactive dictionary feature accessed through hotspots (prompt access to words pronunciation and meanings) emerged as a factor that strengthens word meaning [20,24,50].

Second, as in a previous review [11], we found that the reading of e-books led to modest gains in phonological awareness in favor of the e-book condition.

Third, due to a limited number of studies addressing concepts about print and reading comprehension (see ), no estimates of the practical effect could be presented for these abilities, although positive effects in favor of the e-book are found in both literacy skills.

Concerning the contributions of e-book interventions compared to print books for supporting early literary skills, out of the five reviewed studies, four were found to produce small-to-large size effects in favor of the e-book condition. As in a previous meta-analysis [12], we found that interactive e-book stories showed an advantage of the multimedia element over children encountering traditional print books without receiving adult support. This finding, reported in the reviewed studies [20,44], might suggest that the multimedia features provide similar scaffolding to that of an adult for children’s literacy experiences. Interactive e-books with audio narration allow children to learn vocabulary [20,44] and become aware of sounds [20,41], indicating that using e-books independently may be a valuable practice for promoting children’s acquisition of emergent literacy skills without adult assistance when teachers or parents are otherwise occupied. However, previous research concluded that learning improves when the e-book is shared with an adult [8,9,10,11]; digital literacy activities, such as e-book reading, could ameliorate the lack of adult support during story reading [44] and give educators and parents extra time, which may be needed in the context of distress or crisis, such as the current pandemic situation.

Similarly, all seven reviewed studies that compared enhanced e-book reading interventions to e-book non-enhanced conditions reported non-significant to large effect sizes in favor of enhanced e-book interventions. The e-books selected in the enhanced conditions of the reviewed studies presented interactive characteristics, illustrations, automatic animations, and hotspots that are activated by touching or pressing the touch screen to generate sound and animation [24,42,44,45]. Consequently, the selected e-books are easily accessible for emergent readers who cannot yet decode text or are just beginning to learn to decode it in comparison with non-enhanced, non-interactive, or static e-books. Well-designed and effective educational e-books for young children can serve as a good opportunity to enhance children’s language and literacy [51].

Finally, the six studies comparing e-book interventions versus regular school programs reported quite favorable outcomes of e-book intervention. However, while preschool staff are aware of ways in which literacy might be supported, the learning that can ensue from interactions with technology has not yet been fully recognized, and little attention has been paid to the ways in which children’s emerging competences with technology are supported and can be ameliorated [56]. In fact, of the 14 selected studies for the present review, only one of them [46] is delivered by teachers. Educators and parents are concerned about the perceived negative effects of digital media on children’s health [1]. Nonetheless, guidance on the use of the e-book in schools and at home may be more helpful than restrictions and warnings regarding the risk of young children’s exposure to digital activities and screens.

To address the last aim of the present study, we discuss some aspects of the reviewed studies that are worth mentioning. The first is related to special populations. In the present review, special attention was given to the effects of e-book interventions for the different groups of children by testing every effect separately for disadvantaged and non-disadvantaged children. We found that children at risk of learning disabilities [19,20,21,47], ELL [42], and low SES [51] received a clear positive impact from e-book interventions in all the studied literacy abilities. It is plausible that for children who do not fully understand the story of a book because they lack the language and comprehension skills necessary, non-verbal information from animations and sound effects can bridge the gaps [13]. Additionally, the “app gap” has shrunk substantially, and more children now have access to digital devices [2]; this could create opportunities for children from different backgrounds in online learning conditions, which, for example, has been the case in the present crisis situation.

A second aspect is related to the quality of e-book design and appropriate selection. Although today, e-books, on one hand, have a more educational value for young children compared to those that were on the market a decade ago [16], on the other hand, older types of digital books, which include sound, animations, and interactive hotspots, unlike modern e-books, are operated by a mouse on a desktop computer, which is much less intuitive than modern tablets where children can use their fingers on a touch screen [4]. However, regarding these recent improvements in e-book design, educators and parents need advice about principles of good educational e-books for young children. E-book analyses are needed to make recommendations on what comprises good e-books for young children in order to better serve designers, parents, educators, and children [16].

A third aspect worth mentioning is related to adult implications in e-book interventions. As stated above, the literature base offers preliminary evidence suggesting that adult support may provide additional benefits beyond the interactive features in e-books [8,9,10,11]. However, previous research suggests that young children acquire a wide range of competencies when interacting with technology, which are developed in ways that are not necessarily the result of direct teaching [57]. Consequently, the adult role may move away from direct support while using the e-book to a more distal role, centering on selection and encouragement [3] and the design of appropriate strategies [46] and contexts [21] to learn from e-books.

The fourth aspect refers to the possible negative effects of digital media on children’s health. Internet Gaming Disorder and Gaming Disorder have been included as conditions in the DSM-5 [33] and the ICD-11 [32] respectively, justifying more clinical research and experience on this topic. As Wolf remarks, the reality is that we cannot go back; nor should we move ahead thoughtlessly. In this sense research conducted by E-READ network, New America, the Joan Ganz Cooney Center, and the MacArthur Foundation program encourage educators to help the habits that spur learning no matter where the text comes from, no matter whether the image is on paper or screen [58].

Finally, we would like to consider the limited number of intervention sessions of the reviewed studies, ranging from 2 to 16 sessions. In this sense, one of the selected studies [51] found that the contribution of repeated reading (3 vs. 6 repetitions) contributed to greater vocabulary gains for preschool and kindergarten children after six repetitions compared to three repetitions. Similarly, only one of the reviewed studies [21] presents a follow up of the outcomes. Although the present research findings suggest that reviewed e-book interventions can be effective to increase phonological awareness and vocabulary outcomes among young children, the extent to which gains are generalized and maintained over time has been, at best, scarcely examined. Studies with more intervention and follow-up sessions are needed to better understand the effectiveness of e-book interventions.

Currently, children across all levels of society are using digital technologies and mobile media on a daily basis [1,2,34]. However, research on the educational value of digital literacy for children is still in its early stages. We are only beginning to understand what this digital shift means for young children’s literacy development [3]. However, there is evidence of how children can improve their emergent literacy skills using e-books. Carefully chosen e-books presented on a tablet can offer a highly independent learning experience for young children. As technology becomes increasingly ubiquitous, practitioners and parents must be guided, not only on the frequency and content of the media their child uses, but also on how to introduce media at schools and at home in dedicated spaces and at set times, and how to initiate social and creative ways to use digital media—all of this must take place without displacing their child’s basic needs [34].

With the current health crisis, we have learned that it is possible and highly probable that distance learning involving schools and families assisting children will play a key role in education in the future. Reading is a key academic and life skill that should be encouraged from the early stages of life; not all young children have the opportunity to attend school but can use e-books and digital devices to develop literacy [21]. Therefore, parents and practitioners need a better understanding on how e-books, and what type of e-books, could contribute to the development of emergent literacy skills in young children, taking into account the recommendations given by international organizations to avoid health issues in children [25].

E-Books vs. Print: What Parents Need to Know

The Impact of E-Book Reading on Young Children’s Emergent Literacy Skills: An Analytical Review

306

0

Comments

0/2000

All Comments (0)

Guest Posts

If you are interested in sending in a Guest Blogger Submission,welcome to write for us!

Your Name: (required)

Your Email: (required)

Subject:

Your Message: (required)